Translate

воскресенье, 20 декабря 2015 г.

Personalities that make great seos









Musicians are better at multitasking


A new study suggests that playing music is instrumental to being able to smoothly switch between tasks
In psychology, the term 'task switching' describes the ability to quickly shift your attention between two tasks. Previous studies have suggested that there are many benefits to playing a musical instrument or being bilingual, including enriching mental development and better cognitive function. 
A team of psychologists from York University in Canada were interested in seeing if the skills held by musicians and bilingual individuals could help them with task switching. They predicted both groups would perform better than average, but the results showed that only one outperformed the rest.
The study, published in the journal Cognitive Science, involved 153 students who were separated into four groups - monolingual musicians, bilingual musicians, monolingual non-musicians, and bilingual non-musicians. The musicians had an average of 12 years of formal musical training, and 88 percent of them were instrumentalists. 
The participants were asked to complete a number of tests that measured their ability to switch between two mental tasks. In one activity, they were “required to track a moving white dot (while) at the same time, they attended to single capitalized serif letters flashing one at a time in the centre of the computer screen. Participants were required to click the mouse button whenever they saw the target letter X," says Tom Jacobs from Pacific Standard.
The results showed that the musicians performed much better than non-musicians, but surprisingly, the bilingual participants did not. 
"Musicians' extensive training requires maintenance and manipulation of complex stimuli in memory, such as notes, melody, pitch, rhythm, dynamics, and the emotional tone of a musical piece," write the authors of the study. They report that this meticulous training "may help them to develop superior control to respond efficiently to stimuli in an environment where both switching and non-switching components exist."
More research is yet to be done, but in the meantime, why not tune up on your multitasking skills by learning an instrument.

The first language you learn affects how you hear all others, study finds


New research offers fresh insight into how the first language we learn affects our experience of all the subsequent ones we come across. Scientists in Canada have found that our original language creates certain patterns in the brain that are perhaps never lost.
The influence of this 'locking in' of the brain is still evident even when all knowledge of the first language has disappeared from memory. It appears to inform the way we hear the sounds and words of subsequent languages we come into contact with during our lives, as though our brains have become hardwired in one particular formation or tuned to one particular language.
The team from McGill University and the Montreal Neurological Institute looked at three groups of adolescents: those who only speak French, those who speak both French and Chinese fluently, and adopted teens who originally knew Chinese as babies but now only speak French. MRI scans were used to monitor brain activity while French pseudo-words were played. Interestingly, the same areas of the brain 'lit up' in the bilingual speakers and the monolingual speakers who originally knew Chinese.
In those who had known Chinese at a very early age but are no longer bilingual, their brains handled words as if they still were. The researchers think this could help us understand more about how we learn languages and how that process changes as we get older.
"The adopted children we tested have an interesting background because they were exposed to one language from birth, but completely discontinued that language at a young age when they were adopted into families who speak a different language," said one of the researchers, Lara Pierce. "This is very interesting from a language development perspective because it allows us to look at the influence of just that very early period of language development on later language processing, separately from the effects of ongoing exposure to one or more languages."
It may help explain why learning a language as a baby is so effortless and why it becomes much harder later in life, at least for some of us: our brains have already been 'set' in a certain configuration. As Pierce notes, very young children have been shown to be incredibly adept in picking out words that help in their language learning and dismissing other sounds - a skill we tend to lose in later life.
The study has been published in Nature Communications.